The saying 'it's better to have loved and lost than never
have loved at all' is true, scientists say.
The pain of lost chances lingers on far longer than the
short term regret of doing something wrong according to the study by two U.S.
universities.
And painful as they are, regrets can actually spur us on to
future success, by ensuring that we take chances in the future to avoid missing
out on career or love success.
Researchers identified two different types of regret -
action regrets, based on something which had actually happened, and inaction
regrets, which concerned something a person didn't do but wishes they had.
They surveyed 370 adults, evenly split between genders, who
were asked to discuss one strong regret in detail, whether it was an action or
inaction regret and how long the pain lasted for afterwards.
The most common regrets were about love, education and
work.
Romantic regrets proved particularly painful, with many of
those interviewed dwelling on lost chances for potential romances, or
relationships that didn't work out. And
results, published in the SAGE journal, revealed that action regrets were
initially very painful, but didn't linger as long as inaction regrets.
They also showed that 'love outcomes were regretted more
than career outcomes' and that action regrets were more severe when they
resulted in a big personal loss - such as that of a partner or loved one.
However, the authors also claimed that regrets -
particularly inaction ones - can spur us onto future success, as they drive us
to take chances and make sure we don't suffer the same feeling of missing out
again.
http://www.dailymail.co.uk/sciencetech/article-2001931/Why-IS-better-regret-youve-havent.html#ixzz4Etj4auV5
Regrets of Omission
Another key finding had to do with whether people felt more
regret about actions they did or did not take. Research by Victoria Medvec, a
professor of management and organizations at the Kellogg School, had previously
established a connection between time and regret: The more time that has passed
since an event, the more likely people are to focus on what they failed to do,
rather than what they actually did. “Lost opportunities linger in our memory
longer,” Roese puts it. That is because people can quickly rationalize their
actual actions, even when they went wrong. But for a possible action that was
never taken, “there are so many ways in which you can see different things you
could have done,” he explains.
He illustrates the concept with a romantic example—asking
someone for a date. In remembering an unsuccessful attempt, an unlucky suitor
might think, “I asked this person out on a date, she shot me down, it’s done.”
But if he never even tried, the suitor might ponder all sorts of scenarios:
“What if I had asked her when I saw her in the hallway? What if I had phoned
right after we first met? What if I had sent flowers?”
Roese and Morrison’s new study analyzed people’s feelings
about unrealized actions by asking respondents questions such as, “Does the
regret focus on something you should have done, or something you should not
have done?” and “When did the event happen that made you feel regret?” Through
their broad sampling, Roese and Morrison found that the time disparity, too,
applies to a wide cross-section regardless of race, education level, marital
status, and age.
http://insight.kellogg.northwestern.edu/article/the_biggest_regret_of_all
Morrison M, Roese NJ.
Regrets of the typical American. Findings
from a nationally representative sample.
Social Psychological and Personality Science November 2011 vol. 2 no. 6
576-583.
Abstract
Abstract
In this study of regret among a representative sample of
Americans, the authors examined hypotheses derived from regret regulation
theory, which asserts that regrets motivate a range of ameliorative cognitive
consequences. Using a random-digit telephone survey, respondents reported a
salient regret, then answered questions about that regret. Results showed
inaction regrets lasted longer than action regrets, and that greater loss
severity corresponded to more inaction regrets. Regrets more often focused on
nonfixable than fixable situations. Women more than men reported love rather
than work regrets and, overall, regrets more often focused on romance than on
other life domains. Objective life circumstances (referenced by demographic
variables) predicted regret in patterns consistent with regret regulation
theory. These results complement laboratory findings while suggesting new
refinements to existing theory.
Beike DR, Markman KD, Karadogan F. What we regret most are
lost opportunities:
a theory of regret intensity. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2009
Mar;35(3):385-97.
Abstract
A recent theory (Roese & Summerville, 2005) has
suggested that regret is intensified by perceptions of future opportunity. In
this work, however, it is proposed that feelings of regret are more likely
elicited by perceptions of lost opportunity: People regret outcomes that could
have been changed in the past but can no longer be changed and for which people
experience low psychological closure. Consistent with the lost opportunity
principle, Study 1 revealed that regretted experiences in the most commonly
regretted life domains are perceived as offering the least opportunity for
improvement in the future, Study 2 indicated that people experience the most
regret for outcomes that are not repeatable, and Study 3 revealed that perceptions
of higher past than future opportunities and low psychological closure predict
regret intensity. Discussion focuses on the hope-inducing yet ephemeral nature
of perceived future opportunity and on the relationship between dissonance
reduction and closure.
Sandberg T, Hutter R, Richetin J, Conner M. Testing the role of action and inaction anticipated regret on intentions and behaviour. Br J Soc Psychol. 2016 Mar 25. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12141. [Epub ahead of print]
ReplyDeleteAbstract
Anticipated regret (AR) has been suggested as a useful addition to the theory of planned behaviour (TPB) that captures affective influences. However, previous research has generally (1) assessed the impact of AR in relation to one behaviour (action or inaction) when considering TPB variables in relation to the alternative behaviour, (2) not controlled for affective attitudes or past behaviour, and (3) examined only one or two behaviours. In two studies across several behaviours, the present research showed that even when controlling for affective attitudes, past behaviour, and other TPB variables towards action, action and inaction AR each added to the prediction of intentions across multiple behaviours. The two studies also showed that inaction regret was generally the stronger predictor, although action regret was important for some types of behaviour. Implications and issues for further research are discussed.