Need a good laugh? The feeling is pretty universal,
according to researchers who tickled rats for the sake of science.
When the animals received a 10-second tickle from a gloved
hand, they responded with a rat version of laughter. The animals seemed to
enjoy it — indeed, they ran toward the gloved hand and eagerly chased it around
their enclosure. The Berlin-based researchers even witnessed a joyful jumping
behavior known as “Freudensprünge” that has been observed in other mammals.
Tickling, then, induces a “primitive form of joy” in
animals, said neurobiologist Shimpei Ishiyama of Humboldt University. Ishiyama
and his postdoctoral adviser, Michael Brecht, reported their experiments in
Friday’s edition of the journal Science.
Great thinkers going all the way back to Aristotle in
Ancient Greece have contemplated the mysteries of tickling. Ishiyama and Brecht
outlined some of their own questions.
“Why does tickling induce laughter?” they wrote in their study.
“Why do body parts differ in ticklishness? Why can’t we tickle ourselves?”
To find out, they tickled young male rats in a systematic
way. First, they tickled the animals on the back, then flipped them over and
tickled them on the stomach. That was followed by gentle touching on the back,
then front. Next, the researchers tickled the rats on their tails. Finally,
they played the hand-chasing game. Each part of the routine lasted for about 10
seconds, followed by a 15-second break.
The rats responded with ultrasonic vocalizations in the
range of 50 kilohertz, a pitch with a “positive emotional valence,” according
to the study. That frequency is too high for humans to hear, so the researchers
transposed the vocalizations to lower frequencies. (Notably, none of the
tickles caused rats to make utterances in the 22-kHz range, which are
considered “alarm calls,” according to the study.)
In addition to the vocalizations, the rats also reacted to
tickles with spontaneous Freudensprünge jumps. These jumps resemble bunny hops,
with the front legs and back legs moving in tandem.
Using a suite of electrodes, the scientists found that
tickling prompted a specific pattern of activity in the somatosensory cortex,
the part of the rat brain that processes touch. “Remarkably,” they wrote, the
hand-chasing game activated the same part of the rats’ brains.
Ishiyama and Brecht wondered whether tickled rats would
“laugh” no matter what, or if they had to be in a receptive state of mind. (No
less a scientist than Charles Darwin has proposed that “the mind must be in a
pleasurable condition” for a tickle to work, the pair noted.)
So the researchers made the animals anxious by placing them
on an elevated platform and subjecting them to bright light. Under these
conditions, the rats’ vocalizations were “significantly suppressed.” Brain
activity also was suppressed compared with the normal conditions, the researchers
found.
Later, the scientists were able to make rats laugh without
even touching them. Instead, they stimulated the part of the somatosensory
cortex that was most active when the rats were tickled. That did the trick.
This finding prompted researchers to wonder whether the
somatosensory cortex helps process emotions in addition to tactile sensations.
More work will be needed to answer that question, they wrote.
In the meantime, the results suggest that tickling is more
than a quirk of the nervous system. It may even serve a useful social purpose.
“Tickling might be a trick of the brain to make animals or
humans, respectively, interact and play with each other,” Brecht said.
S. Ishiyama1, M. Brecht.
Neural correlates of ticklishness in the rat somatosensory cortex. Science
11 Nov 2016:Vol. 354, Issue 6313, pp. 757-760.
Abstract
Rats emit ultrasonic vocalizations in response to tickling
by humans. Tickling is rewarding through dopaminergic mechanisms, but the
function and neural correlates of ticklishness are unknown. We confirmed that
tickling of rats evoked vocalizations, approach, and unsolicited jumps
(Freudensprünge). Recordings in the trunk region of the rat somatosensory
cortex showed intense tickling-evoked activity in most neurons, whereas a
minority of cells were suppressed by tickling. Tickling responses predicted
nontactile neural responses to play behaviors, which suggests a neuronal link
between tickling and play. Anxiogenic conditions suppressed tickling-evoked
vocalizations and trunk cortex activity. Deep-layer trunk cortex neurons
discharged during vocalizations, and deep-layer microstimulation evoked
vocalizations. Our findings provide evidence for deep-layer trunk cortex
activity as a neural correlate of ticklishness.
No comments:
Post a Comment